Skip to main content

Theory and practice of social norms interventions: eight common pitfalls

Abstract

Background

Recently, Global Health practitioners, scholars, and donors have expressed increased interest in “changing social norms” as a strategy to promote health and well-being in low and mid-income countries (LMIC). Despite this burgeoning interest, the ability of practitioners to use social norm theory to inform health interventions varies widely.

Main body

Here, we identify eight pitfalls that practitioners must avoid as they plan to integrate a social norms perspective in their interventions, as well as eight learnings. These learnings are: 1) Social norms and attitudes are different; 2) Social norms and attitudes can coincide; 3) Protective norms can offer important resources for achieving effective social improvement in people’s health-related practices; 4) Harmful practices are sustained by a matrix of factors that need to be understood in their interactions; 5) The prevalence of a norm is not necessarily a sign of its strength; 6) Social norms can exert both direct and indirect influence; 7) Publicising the prevalence of a harmful practice can make things worse; 8) People-led social norm change is both the right and the smart thing to do.

Conclusions

As the understanding of how norms evolve in LMIC advances, practitioners will develop greater understanding of what works to help people lead change in harmful norms within their contexts. Awareness of these pitfalls has helped several of them increase the effectiveness of their interventions addressing social norms in the field. We are confident that others will benefit from these reflections as well.

Background

Practitioners and scholars working for global health have long been interested in understanding how socio-cultural context influences people’s health-related behaviour.

Most of the field’s conceptual models acknowledge both the importance of context in shaping behaviour, and the role of social norms. Brofenbenner’s socio-ecological model, for instance, emphasizes how individual, social, institutional, and macro level factors combine to influence people’s actions [1, 2]. The framework invites users to examine how the micro, meso, and macro-level environments interact to affect human behaviour. It has been used to examine a wide range of health practices “e.g. [3,4,5,6,7,8]” and more recently adapted specifically to help design social norms interventions [9]. In addition to the ecological model, several other theories and frameworks exist to study how social factors influence people’s health and health-related behaviours [10]. The fields of medical sociology and social epidemiology, for instance, have offered several paradigms of how “social determinants” combine with an individual’s genetic endowment, and social world to make people either healthy or ill [11]. Approaches that analyse social determinants traditionally evoke factors such as income and income distribution, physical environment, employment and job security, education as well as social networks [12].

When these and other behavioural science models have been used to design health-promotion programmes, the relational dimension of social norms has sometimes been lost; rather interventions have focussed instead on improving knowledge and changing attitudes [13,14,15,16]. Today, we are witnessing an increased interest in using social norms frameworks to inform health promotion interventions in low and mid-income countries (LMIC) [15, 17,18,19,20,21,22,23]. The first “social norms interventions” were originally used to reduce alcohol consumption in a few US universities in the early 1980s by correcting students’ overestimates of how much other students drink [24]. Later, more campuses (mostly in the US, Canada, and the UK) used similar interventions to address tobacco use [25], sexual violence [26, 27], and use of recreational drugs [28].

In the late 90s, several health practitioners working in LMIC began to explore the potential of changing social norms to achieve a wider range of health outcomes, especially around harmful traditional practices. This interest followed the discovery that people in West Africa were abandoning female genital cutting – a non-medically-justified modification of women’s genitalia – in response to interventions that integrated a norms component [29, 30]. Today, many interventions in LMIC aim to change social norms that sustain harmful practices, including, to cite a few examples, child marriage [18, 31, 32], female genital cutting [33, 34] and intimate partner violence [35]. Gelfand and Jackson [36] spoke of this interest as the “emerging science” of social norms, and Miller and Prentice [15] attributed it to a “growing disillusionment with the capacity of factual information and economic inducements to reduce [harmful] behaviour” (p. 340).

Despite this burgeoning interest, the ability of practitioners to use social norm theory to inform health-related interventions varies widely. In practice, we have noticed that when practitioners first apply a social norms frame to behaviour change, they make similar mistakes. To help avoid these early missteps, we outline here, eight common pitfalls that practitioners may encounter in their early efforts to apply social norms theory. We begin my reviewing some of the extensive and multi-disciplinary literature on social norms; we conclude by describing each pitfall in greater depth.

Background

Theoretical and empirical literature on social norms exists in sociology, anthropology, social and moral psychology, economics, law, political science, and health sciences. Definitions across these disciplines vary and sometimes contradict each other. The full range of these definitions includes a constellation of social rules ranging from mere etiquette to the most fundamental moral duties [13, 14, 37, 38]. In their simplest definition, social norms are the informal, mostly unwritten, rules that define acceptable, appropriate, and obligatory actions in a given group or society. Current practitioners’ interest in social norms theory mostly draws from the work by Cialdini and colleagues, who defined social norms as one’s beliefs about: 1) what others in one’s group do (descriptive norms); and 2) what they approve and disapprove of (injunctive norms) [39,40,41,42,43,44]. Influence of norms has been demonstrated empirically. Drawing on Cialdini’s definitions, researchers have demonstrated the influence of social norms on several health-related practices, including: alcohol consumption [45, 46], food intake [47], use of recreational drugs [48, 49], smoking [50], water purification [51], and hand washing [52].

Many theories exist on why people comply with social norms, including when doing so is harmful to self and others. In a recent review, Young [37] identified four main compliance mechanisms: 1) Coordination: people want to achieve a goal that requires coordinated action among group members; to that purpose, they follow what they believe to be common rules for that action; 2) Social pressure: people anticipate social rewards or social punishment for their compliance and non-compliance with a norm; trying to achieve the former and avoid the latter, they follow social norms even when they may prefer not to; 3) Signalling and Symbolism: people want to signal their membership in a given group to self and/or others; to do so, they follow what they think to be the rules specific to that group; 4) Benchmark and Reference points: people internalise rules of what action is considered normal in a given situation, to the point that they follow those rules automatically — what Morris and colleagues called social autopilot in [53].

Social norms are not written in stone; they naturally evolve through time, and sometimes can change very quickly [13, 54,55,56,57,58]. The literature on what works to spark sustainable social norms change in LMIC is still emergent, but growing [16]. In their recent review, Miller and Prentice [15] identified three recurring approaches to change social norms. The first is social norms marketing: this strategy was used in the 80s to address college students’ drinking. To change group behaviour, social norms marketing campaigns aim to correct people’s misperceptions of what others in their group do and approve of. In college drinking interventions, for instance, social marketing strategies sent messages like: “85% of students in this university only drink one beer on Saturday and approve of those who do the same”. The second strategy is personalized normative feedback, where people receive information on how they are performing against others around them. This strategy, which exploits the influence of (descriptive) norms, has been used, for instance, to reduce energy consumption. By telling people whether they were doing better or worse at saving energy than their neighbours, the intervention achieved on average a considerable reduction in energy consumption [59, 60]. The third strategy is facilitator-led group conversations, where participants look critically at existing norms and practices within their group and renegotiate those norms among themselves. A few models exist of how group reflection processes can help achieve change in harmful social norms [18, 20, 61,62,63,64]. Studying effective facilitator-led programmes, Cislaghi [65, 66] identified three steps for social norms change: 1) motivation (where participants learn about the detrimental consequences for themselves and others of their compliance with the harmful norm); 2) deliberation (where participants create a new positive norm within their reference group and identify strategies to motivate others in their surroundings); and 3) action (where participants publicly enact their strategies and motivate others to join the group, eventually reaching the critical mass needed for normative change). This final step is often formalized through a public commitment to change.

The current literature, however, does not adequately elucidate the challenges and potential pitfalls of designing change strategies using social norms theory. Here, we list the most critical factors to be taken into account when designing such interventions.

Eight pitfalls of social norms interventions

As we mentioned, changing health-related social norms is critical to facilitate improvement in people’s relations and wellbeing [67,68,69,70,71,72,73]. Effective health promotion programmes should not only help people resist existing harmful expectations, they should also facilitate change in the expectations around them [70, 74, 75]. We have identified the following eight critically-important pitfalls of social norms interventions that can help design such interventions.

Pitfall #1: Conflating social norms and personal attitudes

The two psychological constructs—social norms and attitudes—are connected but distinct (social norms can influence attitudes and vice versa). One of the most frequently cited social norms theories, Fishbein and Ajzen’s theory of reasoned action, describes attitudes as internally-motivated judgements that people make about something, such as: “I don’t like going to church” [76]. Social norms, instead, are beliefs about what other people do and approve of, for instance, “People around me go to church and people important to me expect me to do likewise”. The difference is important: one person might attend church not because she or he really wants to (attitude), but to meet the expectations of others (see Fig. 1).

Fig. 1
figure 1

A harmful norm can trump one’s protective attitude, resulting in compliance with a harmful practice

The misalignment between attitude and norm can influence the actions of multiple people in a group, to the point that everyone in the group might hold a protective personal attitude (“I believe that girls should be at least 18 before they marry” ) but think that everyone else holds a different position (“the people around me marry their daughters as soon as they reach puberty, and expect me to do likewise”). This phenomenon is commonly referred to as pluralistic ignorance [77, 78]. When most people in a group hold contrasting attitudes and norms, an intervention might achieve change by unveiling the misperception that keeps people bound to the harmful norm. That is, by showing that most people in the group hold the same personal attitudes, interventions might contribute to dismantling the harmful norm.

The difference between attitudes and norms also has implications for social norms measurement. Practitioners who implement an intervention to change social norms should pay attention to the difference between norms and attitudes as they design their measurement strategies, and select some of the tools created specifically to measure social norms “see, for instance: [20, 21]”. Sometimes, however, measures of norms are not available to researchers using existing datasets. Most multi-country datasets (DHS, World Value Survey, MICS, for instance) do not include specific measures of social norms, but they do include measures of personal attitudes (for instance, the DHS includes measures of personal attitudes towards acceptability of violence). Researchers interrogating those datasets often resort to aggregating attitude data at the cluster level, as a proxy for social norms [13, 79, 80]. Note that some researchers have referred to and defined measures of cluster-level attitudes as “collective attitudinal norms” [81, 82].

Pitfall #2: Focussing exclusively on discordant norms and attitudes

A tendency exists in the social norms literature, particularly in social psychology, economics, and implementation science, to focus largely on discordance between attitudes and norms (as depicted in Fig. 1). Since the early work on norms and students’ use of alcohol, a large number of empirical studies investigated how discordant norms and attitudes influence people’s practices “e.g. [28, 29, 83,84,85,86,87,88,89,90,91,92]”. Norms and attitudes, however, can be aligned: not only can people believe that compliance with a harmful practice is expected of them, they can also have a positive personal attitude towards that practice. Take the example of female genital cutting, for instance. In some places, people might think that “cutting their daughter” is both what’s expected of them and a good thing to do independently of what others do (Fig. 2).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Norms and attitudes can be contrasting or aligned

Uncovering the relation between people’s attitudes and norms is critical to intervention design. While correcting misperceptions (as in the campus drinking example) might be an appropriate strategy when norms and attitudes are discordant, it will not serve when people’s attitudes align with the norm. In this latter case, practitioners may first need to change the attitudes of a core group of individuals, and then help them become local change agents, reaching out to the larger group. This might be done, for instance, by providing the group with information on the harmful consequences of a given practice and inviting them to reflect critically on the reasons for the practice. Next, practitioners could help participants devise strategies to motivate others in their settings to join their movement for change (a process that has been referred to as “organised diffusion”) ([30], Cislaghi B, Deeny EK, Cissé M, Gueye P, Shresta B, Shresta P N, Ferguson G, Hughes C, Clark C J: Changing social norms: the importance of “organized diffusion” for scaling up community health promotion interventions, submitted). As the change process starts and the new healthful (or protective) norm spreads within the group, some people’s personal attitudes might not change, but the new norm might induce them to adopt a healthier practice.

In sum, even though there is no universal relationship between attitudes and norms (either may change first), the two nonetheless influence each other in ways that practitioners should study and take account of in their work.

Pitfall #3: Overlooking protective norms

Another implicit bias in development is to see “culture” only as a source of problems rather than as a space for possible solutions. But, in any given cultural context both potentially harmful and potentially protective norms likely exist (See Fig. 3).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Possible effect of attitudes and norms on the practice (assuming the norm trumps the attitude)

As an example, imagine a setting where a norm exists among adolescents under which drinking alcohol is considered a sign of weakness – some readers might have observed such a norm in place in some Scandinavian countries, as well in some of the countries with a quasi-totality of Muslim population (one of the two authors observed this norm to be in place in several areas of Senegal, West Africa). One adolescent might be curious to try alcohol, but fearing community shame might refrain from doing so. As practitioners design their interventions, they would benefit from understanding the protective and harmful roles of existing social norms. Effective interventions might work with local populations in devising strategies to strengthen protective norms, building on existing cultural values and worldviews [93, 94].

Pitfall #4: Assuming social norms are the sole driver of harmful practices

It is rare (although not completely impossible) for social norms to be the exclusive reason motivating people to engage in a harmful action or practice. As many have observed [95,96,97,98,99,100], the ecology of factors contributing to a given practice goes well beyond one specific driver. Understanding how norms intersect with other factors is essential to uncover the pathways that motivate people to compliance with harmful practices. Think, for instance, about the work by Bersamin and colleagues [101], who studied what explains young female students’ lack of access to health services. They did find that norms against using the services could be a possible barrier to access, but they also found that focussing on norms alone wouldn’t be adequate: the services themselves must exist; they must be accessible; and women need to know what services are offered and when they can access them.

In addition to understanding the range of factors that influence a given practice, it is important to understand how they interact. For instance, studying how material and social factors affect people’s electricity consumption, Pellerano and colleagues found that extrinsic financial incentives (a material factor) can sometimes reduce the effect of a normative message (a social factor). Their findings suggest that when people feel that they are complying with a new practice for money, they might be less motivated to do so than when they feel they are complying for a “greater” social purpose [102].

Recently, Cislaghi and Heise [9] offered a practical framework practitioners can use to look at that the ecology of factors contributing to sustaining any given practice. Their framework, which evolves from the well-known ecological framework, includes four domains of influence: institutional, individual, social, and material (see Fig. 4).

Fig. 4
figure 4

Effective interventions uncover and address the interactions between norms and other factors sustaining harmful practices [Source: 9]

Understanding how institutional, individual, social and material factors interact to influence people’s harmful practices can help practitioners design effective interventions that include a social norms perspective. Similar coordinated change could be achieved by organisations collaborating both amongst themselves and with the population whose actions are influenced by these factors.

Pitfall #5: Confusing the prevalence of a social norm with its influence

Partly due to practitioners’ increased efforts to measure social norms as part of programmatic monitoring and evaluation, data on the prevalence of specific norms is starting to appear at conferences, and in reports and academic papers. These data are often presented to explain the extent to which a given norm sustains a particular practice. However, most studies thus far have invested more time, thought, and resources in measuring the prevalence of a norm (i.e. how many people in a specific group hold normative belief X), than measuring its influence (i.e. how many people do X because of the social norm). Scholars of social norms have advanced several hypotheses of what might determine the strength of a norm [14, 19]. Cislaghi and Heise [19], for instance, suggested that characteristics of a practice can affect the influence a norm might exert. One of these characteristics is “detectability”: if a norm exists that “you should do X”, but X is completely undetectable (that is: nobody can know whether others are doing X or not), the norm might have less influence on people’s compliance with X than in the opposite case, when compliance is very detectable.

That is not to say that the relation between a norm X and a related undetectable practice can’t have harmful effects nonetheless. In similar cases, people might never disclose their non-compliance with X, but their non-disclosure might result in harm to self or others. Think of a setting where a social norm exists that says, “you shouldn’t have sex before marriage”. Some adolescents may nevertheless have sex before marriage despite the norm. These same adolescents might not want to disclose to others their sexual activity, possibly anticipating social punishment for it. Their non-disclosure might then limit their capacity to learn about and access modern contraceptive methods (potentially increasing their risk of an unwanted pregnancy or of contracting an STI).

Understanding the influence of one or multiple norms over a given practice should be a priority for effective intervention design. This could be done through qualitative research (see below), possibly coupled with quantitative measures exploring associations between prevalence of normative beliefs and prevalence of the practice of interest, at the cluster level.

Pitfall #6: Neglecting the indirect influence of social norms

Practitioners studying the effect of social norms on a practice X (such as child marriage) might be tempted to look for a norm that people are expected to do X (marry their daughter young). An example comes from female genital cutting, where research conducted in West Africa demonstrated that, in some areas, the practice “cutting your daughter” was sustained by the norm “people around here think that only girls who are cut are respectable” [30, 103, 104]. We call situations where the norm and the behaviour are matched, a direct relation between the practice and the norm [19]. But a practice X can also be indirectly sustained by multiple norms. Intimate partner violence (IPV), for instance, might be sustained by the norms: “you’re not supposed to intervene in another family’s affairs”; “women are not supposed to disclose family matters to others”; and “women are supposed to keep the family together at any cost” (See Fig. 5).

Fig. 5
figure 5

Two possible relations (direct and indirect) between a harmful practice and social norms

Intervention design should thus be informed by exploratory formative research, specifically tailored to develop an in-depth understanding of the relations between various constellations of norms, and their direct and/or indirect effects over the practices of interest. Qualitative strategies and techniques for diagnosing social norms are available elsewhere [20, 21]. Vignettes are often recommended as a good method to diagnose social norms e.g. [13, 105]. Vignettes are short stories that simulate contextual elements of a practice under study (for instance, in the case of child marriage, they might include a father telling his friends that he’s found a husband for his 12-year old daughter). Vignettes are usually followed by a series of questions to probe aspects of the respondent’s beliefs.

Note, however, that vignettes present participants with a set of specific scenarios, relational dynamics, and social contexts in which the action of interest is carried out; in other words, vignettes are selective in the contextual elements they simulate [106]. Their selective nature can be both an advantage and a disadvantage. Since they are selective, vignettes don’t easily allow participants to deviate from the scenario that researchers present to them; researchers have already made choices about who influences who (“the reference group”), the place where the action of interest happens, and the direct or indirect norm that sustains the practice. They are better used when researchers already have an idea of what norms sustain the practice of interest in a particular context. Open-ended techniques are often better suited to situations where little is known about the norms that sustain a given practice. These methods might include participatory approaches that invite participants to discuss all possible contextual elements of the practice of interest. Themes presented to participants might include: in what ways is the action of interest carried out in their context? Who carries it out, who doesn’t and what explains this difference? Where is the action carried out? Who witnesses it? Who would approve or disapprove of it? Vignettes might follow as a strategy to uncover the influence of social norms in a specific number of selected scenarios, built from the previous description of the context in which the action takes place.

Pitfall #7: Publicising the wide prevalence of a harmful social norm

Social norms theory can help recognise the risks in designing campaigns that highlight the great number of people complying with a harmful practice. Those concerned about an issue frequently attempt to motivate change by publicizing the size of the problem: “1 in 3 women globally are abused by their partner”; or: “The average American intakes 44.7 gallons of sugary soda each year”. Because descriptive norms (beliefs about what others do) can influence people’s behaviour, such campaigns can unwittingly reinforce a practice [15, 107, 108]. Even though this point is quite well established in the theoretical and empirical literature, there are still questions about how this finding can inform effective interventions. In designing intervention strategies, caution and serious thought should be given to whether it will be beneficial to use messages that raise awareness in the general population of the size of a problem, like: “65,000 12-year-old girls were married this year in this region alone”. We don’t yet know who is most likely to be influenced by such messages. It might be that these messages sway those who already hold personal attitudes in favour of the harmful practice; but a concrete risk exists that similar messages might backfire, pushing some previous non-compliers to comply with the harmful norm [109].

Pitfall #8: Engineering social norms change from the outside-in

Local worldviews, norms and attitudes intertwine to sustain cultural practices in ways that may be difficult for practitioners to fully decipher in culturally unfamiliar contexts. It can thus be dangerous to design a new desired system of norms from the “outside”. The consequences of the new normative equilibrium might be as harmful as the practices it is meant to replace. Practitioners should thus strive to design people-led interventions that help participants develop both internal motivations to change local norms and strategies to do so in ways that are compatible with the local cultural and social context [65, 110, 111].

Likewise, social norms systems can be highly self-protective. Because those who challenge the norm might face social punishment, their failed attempts to challenge the equilibrium might result in greater harm for them than compliance. Others witnessing this backlash might be discouraged to join future movements for change. Asking people instead to plan and lead the movement for change builds their capacity to identify key change actors, join with them, and then move to action when they feel they have achieved the collaboration of other key people in their network. For the same reason, it might be ineffective (if not dangerous) to spread intervention efforts across geographical or social clusters. Concentrated interventions that work with people’s entire social networks might be both more effective and less likely to elicit backlash against those first agents of change who venture to unsettle the normative equilibrium.

Conclusion

This paper offers some practical reflections for those designing interventions addressing social norms. At this particular moment of research and practice, eight pitfalls seem to be particularly critical for achieving effective normative change. The corresponding learnings for practitioners are: 1) Social norms and attitudes are different; 2) Social norms and attitudes can coincide; 3) Protective norms can offer important avenues for effective social change; 4) Harmful practices are sustained by a matrix of interacting factors; 5) The prevalence of a norm is not necessarily a sign of its strength; 6) Social norms can exert both direct and indirect influence; 7) Publicising the prevalence of a harmful practice can recruit more people to the practice; 8) People-led social norm change is both the right and the smart thing to do. As the understanding of how norms evolve in LMIC advances, practitioners will develop greater understanding of what works to help people lead change in harmful norms within their contexts. Awareness of these pitfalls has helped several programmes in the past increase the effectiveness of their interventions. Hopefully, others will benefit from these reflections as well.

References

  1. Bronfenbrenner U. The ecology of human development. Cambridge: Harvard University Press; 1979.

  2. Bronfenbrenner U. Ecological systems theory. In R. Vasta (Ed.), Six theories of child development: Revised formulations and current issues (pp. 187–249). London: Jessica Kingsley Publishers; 1992.

  3. Smaling EMA. An agro-ecological framework for integrated nutrient management, with special reference to Kenya. 1993, Wageningen University.

  4. DuBois DL, et al. Early adolescent self-esteem: a developmental–ecological framework and assessment strategy. J Res Adolesc. 1996;6:543–79.

    Google Scholar 

  5. Underwood A, Peterson C. Towards an ecological framework for investigating pollution. Mar Ecol Prog Ser. 1988;46:227–34.

    Google Scholar 

  6. Schiamberg LB, Gans D. Elder abuse by adult children: an applied ecological framework for understanding contextual risk factors and the intergenerational character of quality of life. Int J Aging Hum Dev. 2000;50(4):329–59.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Swearer SM, DL. Espelage, Introduction: A Social-Ecological Framework of Bullying Among Youth, in Bullying in American schools: A social-ecological perspective on prevention and intervention, D.L. Espelage and S.M. Swearer, Editors. 2004, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers: Mahwah, NJ, US. p. 1-12.

  8. Heise LL. Violence against women: an integrated, Ecological Framework. Violence Against Women. 1998;4(3):262–90.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Cislaghi B, Heise L. Using social norms theory for health promotion in low-income countries. Health Promot Int. 2018;

  10. Darnton A. Reference Report: An overview of behaviour change models and their uses, in Behaviour Change Knowledge Review. 2008, UK Government Social Research.

  11. Locker D. Social Determinants of Health and Disease, in Sociology as Applied to Health and Medicine, G. Scambler, Editor. 2008, Elsevier: London. p. 18–40.

  12. Braveman P, Gottlieb L. The social determinants of health: it's time to consider the causes of the causes. Public Health Rep. 2014;129(1_suppl2):19–31.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  13. Mackie G, et al. What are social norms? How are they measured? New York: UNICEF and UCSD; 2015. https://www.unicef.org/protection/files/4_09_30_Whole_What_are_Social_Norms.pdf.

  14. Chung A, Rimal RN. Social norms: a review. Rev Commun Res. 2016;2016(4):1–28.

    Google Scholar 

  15. Miller DT, Prentice DA. Changing norms to change behavior. Annu Rev Psychol. 2016;67:339–61.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Tankard ME, Paluck EL. Norm perception as vehicle for social change. Soc Issues Policy Rev. 2016;10(1):181–211.

    Google Scholar 

  17. Nyborg K, et al. Social norms as solutions. Science. 2016;354(6308):42–3.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Vaitla B, Taylor A, Horn JV, Cislaghi B. Social Norms and Girls' Well-Being. New York: United Nations Foundation; 2017.

  19. Cislaghi B, Heise L. Four avenues of normative influence. Health Psychol. 2018;37(6):562–73.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Stefanik L, Hwang T. Applying theory to practice: CARE’s journay piloting social norms measures for gender programming. 2017, CARE USA: Washington DC.

  21. Cislaghi B, Heise L. Measuring Gender-related Social Norms: Report of a Meeting, Baltimore Maryland, June 14-15, 2016. London: London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine; 2017.

    Google Scholar 

  22. Institute of Reproductive health. Learning Collaborative Background Reader. Washington DC, US: Georgetown University; 2016.

  23. Mollen S, Rimal RN, Lapinski MK. What is normative in health communication research on norms? A review and recommendations for future scholarship. Health Commun. 2010;25(6–7):544–7.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Perkins HW, Berkowitz A. Perceiving the community norms of alcohol use among students: some research implications for campus alcohol education programming. Int J Addict. 1986;21:961–76.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Arbour-Nicitopoulos KP, et al. Social norms of alcohol, smoking, and marijuana use within a Canadian University setting. J Am Coll Heal. 2010;59(3):191–6.

    Google Scholar 

  26. Berkowitz, A., The Social Norms Approach to Violence Prevention, in Young Men as Allies in Preventing Violence and Abuse: Building Effective Partnerships with Schools, A. Berkowitz, et al., editors. 2003, FVPF. http://www.alanberkowitz.com/articles/BPI.pdf.

  27. Louise HC, Tricia W, Marissa G. Social norms for sexual violence perpetration in college. J Aggress Conflict Peace Res. 2017;9(4):305–13.

    Google Scholar 

  28. McAlaney J, Jenkins W. Perceived social norms of health behaviours and college engagement in British students. J Furth High Educ. 2015;41(2):1–15.

    Google Scholar 

  29. Mackie G. Ending Footbinding and infibulation: a convention account. Am Sociol Rev. 1996;61(6):999–1017.

    Google Scholar 

  30. Mackie G, LeJeune J. Social Dynamics of Abandonment of Harmful Practices: a New Look at the Theory. Florence: Innocenti Research Centre; 2009.

  31. Freij LS. “Safe Age of Marriage” in Yemen, Fostering Change in Social Norms. Washington DC: USAID; 2010.

  32. Bicchieri C, Jiang T, Lindemans JW. A Social Norms Perspective on Child Marriage: The General Framework. New York: UNICEF; 2014.

    Google Scholar 

  33. UNICEF. Coordinated Strategy to Abandon Female Genital Mutilation/Cutting in One Generation. New York: UNICEF; 2008.

  34. Shell-Duncan B, et al. Dynamics of change in the practice of female genital cutting in Senegambia: testing predictions of social convention theory. Soc Sci Med. 2011;73(8):1275–83.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  35. Trott CD, Harman JJ, Kaufman MR. Women's Attitudes Toward Intimate Partner Violence in Ethiopia: The Role of Social Norms in the Interview Context. Violence Against Women, 2017;23(8):1016–36.

  36. Gelfand MJ, Jackson JC. From one mind to many: the emerging science of cultural norms. Curr Opin Psychology. 2016;8:175–81.

    Google Scholar 

  37. Young HP. The evolution of social norms. Ann Rev Econ. 2015;7(1):359–87.

    Google Scholar 

  38. Bell DC, Cox ML. Social norms: do we love norms too much? J Fam Theory Rev. 2015;7(1):28–46.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Cialdini RB, Reno RR, Kallgren CA. A focus theory of normative conduct: Recyling the concept of norms to reduce littering in public places. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1990;58(6):1015–26.

    Google Scholar 

  40. Kallgren CA, Reno RR, Cialdini RB. A focus theory of normative conduct: when norms do and do not affect behavior. Personal Soc Psychol Bull. 2000;26(8):1002–12.

    Google Scholar 

  41. Cialdini RB, et al. Managing social norms for persuasive impact. Soc Influ. 2006;1(1):3–15.

    Google Scholar 

  42. Goldstein NJ, Cialdini RB. Using social norms as a lever of social influence. In: Pratkanis A, editor. The Science of Social Influence: Advances and Future Progress. Philadelphia: Psychology Press; 2007. p. 167–92.

    Google Scholar 

  43. Schultz PW, et al. The constructive, destructive, and reconstructive power of social norms. Psychol Sci. 2007;18(5):429–34.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Goldstein NJ, Cialdini RB, Griskevicius V. A room with a viewpoint: using social norms to motivate environmental conservation in hotels. J Consum Res. 2008;35(3):472–82.

    Google Scholar 

  45. Prentice DA, Miller DT. Pluralistic ignorance and alcohol use on campus: some consequences of misperceiving the social norm. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1993;64:243–56.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Prestwich A, et al. Does changing social influence engender changes in alcohol intake? A meta-analysis. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2016;84(10):845–60.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Vartanian LR, et al. Modeling of food intake: a meta-analytic review. Soc Influ. 2015;10(3):119–36.

    Google Scholar 

  48. Perkins HW. The social norms approach to preventing school and college age substance abuse: a handbook for educators, counselors, and clinicians. 2003, San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

  49. Jiloha RC. Social and cultural aspects of drug abuse in adolescents. Delhi Psychiatric J. 2009;12(2):167–75.

    Google Scholar 

  50. Eisenberg ME, Forster JL. Adolescent smoking behavior. Am J Prev Med. 2003;25(2):122–8.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Mausezahl D, et al. Solar Drinking Water Disinfection (SODIS) to reduce childhood diarrhoea in Rural Bolivia: A cluster-randomized, controlled trial. PLoS Med. 2009;6(8):e1000125.

  52. Curtis VA, Danquah LO, Aunger RV. Planned, motivated and habitual hygiene behaviour: an eleven country review. Health Educ Res. 2009;24(4):655–73.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  53. Morris MW, et al. Normology: integrating insights about social norms to understand cultural dynamics. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process. 2015;129:1–13.

    Google Scholar 

  54. Campo S, et al. Social norms and expectancy violation theories: assessing the effectiveness of health communication campaigns. Commun Monogr. 2004;71(4):448–70.

    Google Scholar 

  55. de Groot JIM, Schuitema G. How to make the unpopular popular? Policy characteristics, social norms and the acceptability of environmental policies. Environ Sci Pol. 2012;19-20:100–7.

    Google Scholar 

  56. Marcus R. Changing discriminatory norms affecting adolescent girls through communications activities: insights for policy and practice from an evidence review. London: ODI; 2014.

    Google Scholar 

  57. Watson C, Harper C. How communications can change social norms around adolescent girls. London: ODI; 2016.

    Google Scholar 

  58. Cardoso LF, Sorenson SB. Violence Against Women and Household Ownership of Radios, Computers, and Phones in 20 Countries. Am J Public Health. 2017;107:e1–7.

    Google Scholar 

  59. Allcott H. Social norms and energy conservation. J Public Econ. 2011;95(9–10):1082–95.

    Google Scholar 

  60. Ayres I, Raseman S, Shih A. Evidence from two large field experiments that peer comparison feedback can reduce residential energy usage. J Law Econ Org. 2012;29(5):992–1022.

    Google Scholar 

  61. Abramsky T, et al. Findings from the SASA! Study: a cluster randomized controlled trial to assess the impact of a community mobilization intervention to prevent violence against women and reduce HIV risk in Kampala, Uganda. BMC Med. 2014;12:122.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  62. Michau L, et al. Prevention of violence against women and girls: lessons from practice. Lancet. 2015;385:1672–84.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Vu L, et al. Inequitable gender norms from early adolescence to young adulthood in Uganda: tool validation and differences across age groups. J Adolesc Health. 2017;60(2S2):S15–21.

  64. Cislaghi B, Gillespie D, Mackie G. Expanding the aspirational map, in human rights education: theory, research, praxis, M. Bajaj and N. Flowers, Editors. 2017, University of Pennsylvania Press: Philadelphia. p. ix, 353 pages.

  65. Cislaghi B. Human rights and community-led development. Oxford: EUP; 2017.

    Google Scholar 

  66. Cislaghi B. The story of the ‘now-women’: changing gender norms in rural West Africa. Dev Pract. 2018;28(2):257–68.

    Google Scholar 

  67. Gallagher KE, Parrott DJ. What accounts for men's hostile attitudes toward women? The influence of hegemonic male role norms and masculine gender role stress. Violence Against Women. 2011;17(5):568–83.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  68. Arango DJ, et al. Interventions to Prevent or Reduce VAWG - A Systematic Review of Reviews. Washington: World Bank; 2014.

    Google Scholar 

  69. Jakobsen H. What’s gendered about gender-based violence?: an empirically grounded theoretical exploration from Tanzania. Gend Soc. 2014;28(4):537–61.

    Google Scholar 

  70. Jewkes R, Flood M, Lang J. From work with men and boys to changes of social norms and reduction of inequities in gender relations: a conceptual shift in prevention of violence against women and girls. Lancet. 2015;385(9977):1580–9.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Madhani FI, et al. Women’s perceptions and experiences of domestic violence: an observational study from Hyderabad, Pakistan. J Interpers Violence. 2015;32(1):76–100.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  72. Temmerman M. Research priorities to address violence against women and girls. Lancet. 2015;385(9978):e38–40.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Scott MA, Bell E, Holden J. Shifting social norms to tackle violence against women and girls. London: DFID; 2016.

    Google Scholar 

  74. Barker G, et al. Questioning gender norms with men to improve health outcomes: evidence of impact. Glob Public Health. 2010;5(5):539–53.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Cislaghi B, Gillespie D, Mackie G. Values Deliberation and Collective Action: Community Empowerment in Rural Senegal. New York: Palgrave MacMillan; 2016.

  76. Fishbein M, Ajzen I. Belief, Attitude, Intention, and Behavior: An introduction to theory and research. 1975, Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.

  77. Miller DT, McFarland C. Pluralistic ignorance: when similarity is interpreted as dissimilarity. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1987;53:298–305.

    Google Scholar 

  78. Prentice DA, Miller DT. Pluralistic ignorance and the perpetuation of social norms by unwitting actors. Adv Exp Soc Psychol. 1996;28:161–209.

  79. Linos N, et al. Influence of Community Social Norms on Spousal Violence: A Population-Based Multilevel Study of Nigerian Women. Am J Public Health. 2013;103:1.

    Google Scholar 

  80. Storey D, Kaggwa EB. The Influence of Changes in Fertility Related Norms on Contraceptive Use in Egypt, 1995–2005. Popul Rev. 2009;48:1.

    Google Scholar 

  81. Lapinski MK, Rimal RN. An explication of social norms. Communication Theory. 2005;15(2):127–47.

    Google Scholar 

  82. Stark L, et al. How gender- and violence-related norms affect self-esteem among adolescent refugee girls living in Ethiopia. Global Mental Health. 2018;17:5.

    Google Scholar 

  83. Silva A, John P. Social norms don't always work: an experiment to encourage more efficient fees collection for students. PLoS One. 2017;12(5):e0177354.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  84. Taylor CA, et al. Perceived social norms, expectations, and attitudes toward corporal punishment among an urban community sample of parents. J Urban Health. 2011;88(2):254–69.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  85. Neighbors C, et al. Normative misperceptions of abuse among perpetrators of intimate partner violence. Violence Against Women. 2010;16(4):370–86.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  86. Ganz G, Neville FG, Ward CL. Applying behavioural science to issues of public health in South Africa: the case for social norms intervention. S Afr J Sci. 2017;113(5/6):1–5.

    Google Scholar 

  87. Borsari B, Carey KB. Descriptive and injunctive norms in college drinking: a Meta-analytic integration. J Stud Alcohol. 2003;64(3):331–41.

  88. Berger C, Caravita SC. Why do early adolescents bully? Exploring the influence of prestige norms on social and psychological motives to bully. J Adolesc. 2016;46:45–56.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  89. Haylock L, et al. Shifting negative social norms rooted in unequal gender and power relationships to prevent violence against women and girls. Gend Dev. 2016;24(2):231–44.

    Google Scholar 

  90. Read-Hamilton S, Marsh M. The communities Care programme: changing social norms to end violence against women and girls in conflict-affected communities. Gend Dev. 2016;24(2):261–76.

    Google Scholar 

  91. Bhattacharjee P. Honour and Prestige: The influence of social norms on violence against women and girls in Karnataka, South India. London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine: London; 2017.

  92. Hackman CL, Witte T, Greenband M. Social norms for sexual violence perpetration in college. J Aggress Conflict Peace Res. 2017;9(4):301–13.

    Google Scholar 

  93. Iwelunmor J, et al. Toward the sustainability of health interventions implemented in sub-Saharan Africa: a systematic review and conceptual framework. Implement Sci. 2016;11:43.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  94. Gillespie D, Melching M. The transformative power of democracy and human rights in nonformal education: the case of Tostan. Adult Educ Q. 2010;60(5):477–98.

    Google Scholar 

  95. Wyer RSJ. The automaticity of everyday life. Advances in social cognitition. vol. X. New York: Psychology Press; 2014.

  96. Kwasnicka D, et al. Theoretical explanations for maintenance of behaviour change: a systematic review of behaviour theories. Health Psychol Rev. 2016;10(3):277–96.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  97. Grant C. Behaviour change approaches for social norms regarding gender. K4D Helpdesk Report. Brighton, UK: Institute of Development Studies; 2017.

  98. Lilleston PS, et al. Understanding social norms and violence in childhood: theoretical underpinnings and strategies for intervention. Psychol Health Med. 2017;22(sup1):122–34.

  99. Heise L. Violence against women: an integrated, Ecological Framework. Violence Against Women. 1998;4(3):262–90.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  100. May C. Towards a general theory of implementation. Implement Sci. 2013;8(18):1–14.

    Google Scholar 

  101. Bersamin M, et al. Reproductive Health Services: Barriers to Use Among College Students. J Community Health. 2016;42(1):155–9.

    Google Scholar 

  102. Pellerano JA, et al. Do extrinsic incentives undermine social norms? Evidence from a field experiment in energy conservation. Environ Resour Econ. 2016;67(3):413–28.

    Google Scholar 

  103. Heise L. What works to prevent partner violence? An evidence overview. London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine - STRIVE.

  104. McChesney K. Successful approaches to ending female genital cutting.Journal of Sociology & Social Welfare 2015;42(1).

  105. Bicchieri, C., Why people do what they do: a social norms manual for Zimbabwe and Swaziland. 2015, UNICEF: Florence, Italy.

  106. Hughes R, Huby M. The application of vignettes in social and nursing research. Methodol Issues Nurs Res. 2001;37(4):382–6.

    Google Scholar 

  107. Reid AE, Cialdini RB, Aiken LS. Social Norms and Health Behavior. In: Steptoe A, editor. Handbook of Behavioral Medicine. New York: Springer; 2010. p. 263–71.

    Google Scholar 

  108. Bosson JK, et al. A dangerous boomerang: injunctive norms, hostile sexist attitudes, and male-to-female sexual aggression. Aggress Behav. 2015;41(6):580–93.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  109. Goldstein NJ, Mortensen CR. Social Norms: A How-To (and How-Not-To) Guide. In: Kenrick DT, Goldstein NJ, Braver SL, editors. Six degrees of social infuence : science, application, and the psychology of Robert Cialdini. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2012.

    Google Scholar 

  110. Bajaj M, Cislaghi B, Mackie G. Advancing transformative human rights education. In: Brown G, editor. The universal declaration of human rights in the 21st century. Cambridge: OPB; 2016.

  111. Ibrahim S. How to build collective capabilities: the 3C-model for grassroots-led development. J Hum Dev Capabilities. 2017;18(2):197–222.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We wish to thank Giulia Ferrari (LSHTM), Erin Stern (LSHTM), and Rebecca Liron (Columbia University) who provided feedback on an earlier version of this manuscript.

Funding

Cislaghi’s time was funded by an anonymous donor. The study was supported the STRIVE research programme consortium, funded by UKaid from the Department for International Development. However, the views expressed do not necessarily reflect the department’s official policies.The funding body did not have any role in the ideation and writing of the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

BC Drafted and reviewed the manuscript and was the main contributor to conception and design. LH Reviewed and commented on the manuscript and made substantial contributions to conception and design. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Beniamino Cislaghi.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Not applicable.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Cislaghi, B., Heise, L. Theory and practice of social norms interventions: eight common pitfalls. Global Health 14, 83 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12992-018-0398-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12992-018-0398-x

Keywords